[Editors] MIT shows how brain tells glossy from grainy surfaces
Elizabeth Thomson
thomson at MIT.EDU
Thu Apr 19 15:27:25 EDT 2007
MIT News Office
Massachusetts Institute of Technology
Room 11-400
77 Massachusetts Avenue
Cambridge, MA 02139-4307
Phone: 617-253-2700
http://web.mit.edu/newsoffice/www
======================================
MIT shows how brain tells glossy from grainy surfaces
--Work could lead to better robotic vision
======================================
For Immediate Release
THURSDAY, APR. 19, 2007
Contact: Elizabeth A. Thomson, MIT News Office
Phone: 617-258-5402
Email: thomson at mit.edu
PHOTO, GRAPHIC AVAILABLE
CAMBRIDGE, Mass.--Imagine looking at a pool of spilled milk. Your
brain knows that it's milk and not another white substance like
sugar, or cottage cheese, but how does it know?
MIT researchers and colleagues investigating how the brain interprets
the appearance of surfaces think they have an answer. They have found
that the perception of reflectance and gloss are correlated with
certain statistical properties of the image. These properties could
be coded by neurons that respond differentially to light and dark
spots.
The research team, a collaboration between MIT and the NTT
Communication Science Labs in Japan, reported its findings in the
April 18 online issue of Nature.
Studying how the brain analyzes surface appearance is not only
important to understanding the workings of the human brain, but could
also help scientists develop better visual systems for robots.
"We know a lot about the perception of objects, but much less about
the perception of the materials that the objects are made of," said
Edward Adelson, an author of the paper and professor of visual
science in the Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences at MIT.
"Our studies show that statistical skewness, which quantifies an
asymmetry between light and dark patterns, has a strong influence on
the way a material is perceived."
Analyzing visual attributes such as color, texture, and gloss, is
critical in everyday tasks such as deciding whether a patch of
pavement is icy, whether a pancake is cooked, or whether skin is
healthy, according to the researchers.
In their experiments, the research team asked subjects to rate the
lightness and glossiness of natural materials such as stucco or
fabric. The surfaces have a mix of dark shadows and bright
highlights, giving rise to distinctive visual patterns.
"Natural surfaces are complicated," said Adelson. "They have bumps
and dips, and the light reflects in complex ways, producing
characteristic statistical patterns." These patterns serve as
signatures both for the shape and the material composing the surface.
The researchers quantified the images in terms of "luminance
histograms," which plot the distribution of pixel values. They also
estimated the histograms of responses of neurons that respond to
light and dark spots in an image. In both cases, they found that the
"skewness" of the histogram, which measures its asymmetry, was
correlated with the subject's perceptions of surface qualities.
Positive skewness led to darker and glossier surface appearance.
The researchers also found that they could manipulate subjects'
perceptions of glossiness by digitally manipulating the skewness of
the images, said study author Lavanya Sharan, an MIT graduate student
at the Computer Science and Artificial Intelligence Lab.
Technology based on this research could also be useful for autonomous
vehicles, said Sharan.
"You want to know what kind of surface you're on-is the road dry or
wet or icy? Are you on a dirt road? A machine vision system needs to
make these judgments based on the surface appearance," she said.
The exact neural mechanism for detecting image skewness is unknown,
but the researchers have offered a tentative model.
In the retina and brain, there are cells that are preferentially
sensitive to either bright patterns or dark ones. After pooling
responses within each cell population, the brain could compare the
balance between very bright and very dark patterns. This balance
determines the skewness.
"We don't know what the underlying physiology is, but what we do know
is that the computation of skewness is something that could be done
very easily with the hardware that does exist in the early parts of
the brain's visual system," said Adelson.
The researcher's skewness model is supported by experiments with
visual aftereffects, which showed that the human visual system can
adapt to skewness. Similar to what happens when you stare at an image
of a certain color and then see an after-image, staring at patterns
with a high degree of positive skewness will cause the next pattern
you look at to appear negatively skewed, which causes the material to
appear lighter and less glossy.
The lead author on the Nature paper is Isamu Motoyoshi of the Human
and Information Science Lab at NTT Communication Science Labs in
Japan. Shin'ya Nishida of NTT is also an author on the paper.
The research was funded by NTT (Nippon Telegraph and Telephone
Corporation) and the National Science Foundation.
--END--
Written by Anne Trafton, MIT News Office
More information about the Editors
mailing list